Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.
Advertisement
Scientific Reports volume 12, Article number: 4172 (2022)
1650
Metrics details
This systematic review and meta-analysis was conducted to investigate the effectiveness of behavioral sleep interventions (BSIs) on the number of child night awakenings, and maternal sleep quality and depression. The search followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses guidelines (PRISMA) using PubMed, CINAHL, Cochrane, and EMBASE databases and retrieved studies published until April 2021. We calculated the odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for child sleep problems, and the mean differences (MD) and 95% CI for the number of child night awakenings, and maternal sleep quality and depression. Ten studies of 1628 initial searched were included in the final analysis. Two of the 10 studies were divided into two subgroups by participants and intervention type; thus, 12 subgroups were included in the meta-analysis. BSIs significantly reduced child sleep problems (OR 0.51; 95% CI 0.37–0.69) and improved maternal sleep quality (MD − 1.30; 95% CI − 1.82 to − 0.77) in the intervention group. There were no significant differences in the number of child night awakenings and maternal depression between the two groups. More RCTs to examine the effect of BSIs considering children’s age, duration of intervention, and outcome measuring time points are needed.
Sleep problems are common in young children and occur in more than 20% of infants and toddlers1,2. These can adversely affect both children and mothers as the main caregivers of children. Sleep problems affect young children’s cognitive development3,4,5, behavior3,6, quality of life7, and place them at increased risk of obesity8,9. Previous studies found that 75% of children aged 0–4 years old had frequent night awakenings10, and infants who were short and persistent night-time sleepers could not integrate their sleep cycles until they were two or three years old11. Young children who slept for a short duration at night were five times more likely to show hyperactivity12. Young children’s sleep problems also affect their mothers’ physical and emotional health, and can cause insufficient maternal sleep duration, maternal fatigue, parenting stress, and depression1,13,14,15,16. These conditions can affect breastfeeding13,15, and lead to a lack of mother–child bonding, low parenting quality14, and poor development in children17.
The circadian rhythm is a natural internal process that regulates the sleep–wake pattern and repeats roughly every 24 h in humans18. Although this timing system is present during the fetal period, it is strongly influenced by the maternal circadian rhythm in the uterine environment19. At birth, being separated from the mother’s rhythm, the newborn will already possess an immature circadian rhythm, but this will still dramatically change over the first year of life20. The critical factor in developing an infant’s immature 24-h circadian rhythm is the social interaction between mother and child21. For example, mothers provide fundamental care—from feeding to sleeping—to manage an infant’s daily routine by controlling the physical home environment day and night. However, these maternal roles are played in irregular sleep–wake patterns in mothers. The chief complaint of mothers in the 3–4-month postpartum period was fatigue due to irregular sleep–wake patterns as a result of caring for their babies22. Factors interfering with the establishment of regular circadian rhythms in children are associated with inconsistent interactions between parents and children and irregular sleep–wake patterns23. Therefore, behavioral sleep interventions (BSIs) that focus on establishing a stable circadian rhythm within the first year after birth known as critical periods are needed.
BSI for young children, such as settling and bedtime routines, are widely used training methods to improve sleep quality in children with sleep problems such as frequent night-time awakenings, short sleep duration, and sleep-onset latency. The settling methods are carried out in various ways, such as cry it out and fading24,25. The common effect of these methods is to allow children to self-soothe. These interventions provide guidance on sleep-related parenting behavior, such as delayed response to their baby’s signal or nocturnal cries, e.g.: extinction, graduated extinction methods under parental presence, encouraging self-soothing, and falling asleep independently. The settling intervention can help develop a child’s self-regulation abilities in preparation for the transition to a separate space without parental intervention. Another method to help children develop routine sleep habits is by regularly recognizing a bedtime26,27. Bedtime routines generally include sleep hygiene, such as bathing or relaxing massage. Other routines include regulating sleep, feeding, reading a book, singing a lullaby together, and other strategies to induce sleep in the baby. Previous studies reported that BSIs have resulted in a one-third reduction in sleep latency and fewer night awakenings—2.2 to 0.1 in the intervention group compared to the control group with no change28. These interventions also have been found to significantly improve sleep quality in parents24,25,26,27, children’s cognitive skills29, and parental mental health24,30,31. Furthermore, these BSIs can also be helpful for family functioning by reducing maternal distress and increasing marital satisfaction32.
Although a few systematic reviews (SR) related to the effectiveness of sleep interventions have been conducted, there were limitations, such as the lack of objectivity in synthesizing results, interventions being too broadly defined, or the small number of included studies. Thus, the effectiveness of BSIs for children remains unclear. For example, one SR33 did not fully follow the Preferred Reporting Items for SR and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines34, regarding the selection process of the included studies and risk of bias in individual studies. The quality of assessment is an essential step in SR and can lead to adverse consequences if not appropriately performed35. Regarding the scope of the definition of sleep intervention, one SR included both a child sleep intervention and the mother’s psychosocial intervention (i.e., relaxation technique)36 and another SR study reported the synthesis of only two studies37.
Therefore, the purpose of this study was to evaluate behavioral sleep interventions for settling or bedtime routines of children aged 36 months or below. The goal was to identify the efficacy of BSIs in improving children’s sleep problems, improving maternal sleep quality and reducing depression, and reducing the frequency of children’s night awakenings.
A SR and meta-analysis was performed according to the guidelines of the recently updated PRISMA guidelines34. The protocol for this SR was registered on PROSPERO (registration number: CRD42021229365) and is available on the PROSPERO website (https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/).
This study was a SR and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials (RCTs) to verify the effects of BSIs in children.
Electronic searches were conducted on the PubMed, Embase, CINHAL, and Cochrane databases from their inception to April 30, 2021.
The articles were selected in two steps. First, the titles and abstracts of the articles found through the search engines were reviewed by two independent researchers to eliminate articles that were not suitable for the current study. Second, the full text of the articles selected in the first step were then reviewed in order to obtain specific information on study design, participants, and interventions. Finally, the inclusion of studies that were originally disagreed upon by the researchers was determined through discussion.
The database searches were limited to articles published in English, with human subjects, and which utilized RCTs by using the following Medical Subject Headings (MeSH) and keyword MeSH term and/or keywords in combinations: child, sleep, behavior, intervention, mothers, and depression. The search strategy is presented in detail in the Supplementary Table 1.
The subsequent inclusion criteria based on the PICO approach (population [P], intervention [I], comparators [C], outcome [O]) were used: (1) participants were young children aged 36 months or below; (2) interventions included BSIs based on self-settled, self-regulation, and bedtime routines; (3) comparators were the control groups who applied usual care and who did not receive BSIs; (4) outcome measures included child night awakenings, child sleep problems, maternal depression, and maternal sleep quality; and (5) RCTs designs published in a peer-reviewed journal and published in English. Exclusion criteria were (1) studies that were published in gray literature, such as abstracts and/or those that were not original articles, and (2) follow-up studies after an initial study.
BSIs were defined as follows: First, settling intervention (i.e., “self-settling” or “self-soothing” method) for child’s sleep was defined as a practice that aimed to focus on consolidating sleep and extending night sleep duration. Second, bedtime routine interventions was defined as a series of consistent and repetitive activities that was carried out before bedtime every night. These interventions have no standard components since they depend on the developmental stage of the children and the cultural situation. Therefore, bedtime routine interventions was defined as a practice that consisted of more than two activities before going to sleep.
All extracted outcome variables in the included studies are presented in detail in Supplementary Table 2. Studies that evaluated two specific aspects were chosen. The first aspect was child night awakenings and sleep problems reported by mothers using a sleep diary for their children. The second aspect was maternal sleep quality measured by the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI), and maternal depression measured by the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) for mothers.
The Risk of Bias 2 (RoB2) tool was used to assess the quality of appraisal in this SR. The RoB2 tool is a revised version of the original risk of bias tool that aimed to overcome certain limitations of the original version38. The RoB2 is structured according to five domains (randomization process, deviations from intended interventions, missing outcome data, measurement of the outcome, selection of the reported result), and overall bias. Judgment can be at “low” or “high risk” or can express “some concerns.” After two researchers independently assessed the risk of bias using the RoB2, they cross-checked the results and reached an agreement through discussions with each other about any disagreements or unclear judgments.
The data were extracted and converted into an excel file based on the Cochrane recommendations39: publication year, country, study design, sample size of each group, participants’ characteristics including mean and range of age, contents of intervention, duration and frequency of intervention, and main outcomes.
The numerical values, such as the mean and standard deviation (SD), of the main outcomes (i.e., child night awakenings, maternal sleep quality, and maternal depression) were determined, as well as the sample size at certain time points after the intervention (within 1 to 4 months). The main outcome of sleep problems in children as recorded by their mothers in a sleep diary was event rate and the sample size at the aforementioned time points after the intervention.
Mean and SD were used for continuous variables such as child night awakenings, maternal sleep quality, and depression scores. The odds ratio and 95% confidence interval were calculated as data synthesis statistics for dichotomous data, such as the child sleep problems for dichotomous data, such as the child sleep problems. For three of the studies40,41,42, an email was sent to the corresponding author of each in order to request for additional data. Additionally, 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were used in the calculation of the effect size of the overall mean and each effect size. Review Manager (RevMan) version 5.3 was used for meta-analysis. A homogeneity test between studies was performed using chi-square tests, and I2 tests. Meta-analysis using a random-effect model is appropriate for integrating intervention studies. These include different interventions, different results of ROB assessments and different participants’ characteristics43,44,45. Studies included in meta-analysis have heterogeneity. Therefore, all subgroup analyses were applied to the random-effect model.
The selection process of the studies was based on the PRISMA guidelines as illustrated in Fig. 1. A total of 1628 studies were initially searched: 644 studies from PubMed, 470 studies from Embase, 216 studies from CINHAL, and 298 studies from Cochrane. Of these, 82 studies were excluded due to duplication. Among the remaining 1546 studies, 1513 studies were excluded because they were not related to the PICO of this study as determined by the title and abstract of each study. After 33 full-text studies were reviewed, 23 studies were excluded according to specific exclusion criteria: non-English language (n = 1); irrelevant participants (n = 9); irrelevant intervention (n = 4); and non-RCTs design (n = 9), which included one mixed-method study, one second-data analysis, one study protocol, and six quasi-experimental designs using one group pre-post test. Finally, 10 studies were included in this study24,30,31,40,41,42,46,47,48,49. However, two of these studies were divided into two subgroups based on their participants and intervention type41,47; thus, a total of 12 subgroup analyses were included in the meta-analysis.
Flow chart of study selection.
The characteristics of the included studies are summarized in Table 1. The studies were published in 2002, 2007, 2009, 2011, 2013, 2017, 2018, and 2021, while two were published in 2016. Four studies were conducted in the United States, three studies in Australia, and one each in Canada, New Zealand, and Iran. The age of the children who participated in these studies ranged from 0 to 36 months. Regarding the intervention, five studies used settling methods, four studies used bedtime routines, and one study used both. In all the studies, the control group received usual care. The intervention periods were 2 weeks (three studies), 4 weeks, 8 weeks, 40 weeks, and from prenatal to 3 weeks after birth; the intervention period was not given in three studies. The time for assessing outcome measurements was up to 12 months after completing the intervention. Regarding the outcome variables, the presence of child sleep problems (two studies), the number of child night awakenings (eight studies), maternal depression measured by EPSD (three studies), and maternal sleep quality measured by PSQI (three studies) were used to evaluate the effect of BSIs on children.
The results of the quality assessment using RoB2 are presented in Fig. 2. and Table 2. The overall bias was “high risk” in one study, “some concerns” in three studies, and “low risk” in six studies. In the randomization process item, two studies were judged to have “some concerns” because they randomly assigned study participants and did not mention a specific allocation sequence or allocation concealment. Another study was classified under “some concerns” due to deviations from the intended interventions and because the study described the impossibility of blinding. The risk of bias in the selection of the reported results was assessed as “high risk” in another study because it reported the outcome based on meaningful results by means of converting categorical variables. However, using the other RoB2 evaluation criteria, all of the included studies were judged to be low risk.
Risk of bias graph: based on researchers’ opinions about each risk of bias item, percentages are presented for all included studies.
The effects of BSIs on child sleep problems are presented in Fig. 3a. Studies have reported the presence of child sleep problems in both the intervention and control groups even after BSIs. These studies were homogeneous with an I2 value of 5%. The odds ratio of child sleep problems was 0.51 (95% CI 0.37–0.69; p < 0.00001; k = 5), which showed that BSIs significantly reduced child sleep problems in the intervention group compared to the control group.
Forest plot comparing the effects of behavioral sleep intervention. (a) Child sleep problems reported by mothers, (b) Child night awakenings, (c) Maternal sleep quality, and (d) Maternal depression.
The effect of BSIs on the number of child night awakenings is shown in Fig. 3b. Studies reported the number of night awakenings in both intervention and control groups after the BSIs and were heterogeneous with I2 = 66%. The mean difference in the number of child night awakenings was − 0.17 (95% CI − 0.37–0.03; p = 0.10; k = 10), which showed no significance between groups.
The effects of BSIs on maternal sleep quality are shown in Fig. 3c. Studies have reported the maternal sleep quality of both the intervention and control groups using PSQI after BSIs. These studies were homogeneous with an I2 value of 1%. The mean difference in maternal sleep quality was − 1.30 (95% CI − 1.82 to − 0.77; p < 0.00001; k = 3), which indicates that the BSIs significantly improved the maternal sleep quality of the intervention group compared to the control group.
The effect of BSIs on maternal depression is shown in Fig. 3d.
Studies reported maternal depression in both intervention and control groups using EPSD after BSIs and were homogeneous with I2 = 13%. The mean difference in maternal depression was − 0.22 (95% CI − 0.68–0.25, p = 0.33; k = 7), indicating that there was no significant difference between groups.
This study was conducted to examine the effects of BSIs on the sleep of children and mothers. As a result of the previous study search, 10 RCTs published from 2002 to 2021 were included, and significant effects of the BSIs on child sleep problems and maternal sleep quality were found.
In children’s aspects, when comparing BSIs with usual care, BSIs significantly decreased child sleep problems as reported by their parents. Parent-reported child sleep can provide integrative information on environmental and behavioral dimensions and is the most commonly used method for measuring children’s sleep50,51. Parent-reported problematic sleep is a strong predictor of sleep problems in children41. Child sleep problems vary according to developmental stage, are common in early childhood, and decline with age52. Williamson et al. examined sleep behaviors from birth to middle childhood using a longitudinal research design over a ten-year period. They found that reports of child sleep problems by caregivers were most prevalent in the infant stage and reduced with age, and the most common problem from infancy to seven years old was night awakening53. Focusing on infants and toddlers, Mindell et al. investigated the development of infant and toddler sleep patterns and found that infant sleep patterns more clearly develop at 5–6 months. That is, night-time sleep starts to integrate at 5 months and stabilizes more clearly at 6 months54. These changes in children, especially in infants’ sleep problems and patterns, are due to maturation of the circadian system. Sleep–wake homeostasis is related to multiple parenting and environmental factors, such as feeding, exposure to light, and sound50. Therefore, BSIs for infants should be considered at this critical time (i.e., 5–6 months after birth).
The duration of a behavioral sleep intervention in addition to the child’s developmental stage could affect sleep outcomes. That is, a subgroup analysis of the effect of BSIs by children’s age and intervention duration may be meaningful for detailed practical implications. However, the number of studies included in the current study was so small that subgroup analyses could not be performed. Therefore, more RCTs on children of varied developmental stages and with varied intervention durations are needed to further examine the effects of BSIs. With the accumulation of such RCT research, more specific and practical applications of BSIs can be expected.
The child night awakenings did not significantly change given the type of behavioral sleep intervention. A similar result was found in a SR and meta-analysis of infants aged under 12 months37. The number of night awakenings is one of the most commonly used methods for measuring sleep problems55, but night awakenings may not actually be sleep problems in children, especially infants. A possible interpretation of these results may be related to the participants’ developmental characteristics. That is, regardless of BSIs, night-time sleep awakenings could normally occur in infants due to feeding and diaper change56,57, and their immature circadian system. Moreover, parents of breastfed infants have reported more frequent night awakenings and shorter night-time sleep periods58. Among the studies included in the current study, there were eight studies used night awakenings as an outcome variable. Although seven studies of them included infants under 12 months of age, only two studies consider night feeding49 and feeding type40, and most of the studies did not fully consider participants’ developmental characteristics. Therefore, future research needs to consider how to measure infants’ night awakenings caused by sleep problems,s not by feeding or diaper change.
Although more studies on sleep have measured sleep patterns or problems using actigraphy in recent years, we used parent-reported child sleep as one of the outcome variables because most of the included studies utilized it. It is known that parent-reported sleep problems are easily measured, especially for infants and toddlers, but this method may overestimate sleep duration compared to actigraphy51,59. Actigraphy also has its limitations. For instance, it overestimates the number of awakenings since it can measure normal motor activity during sleep as a night awakening60. To consider the benefits and limitations of parent-reported data and actigraphy, the concurrent use of subjective and objective methods could be the best way to evaluate child sleep problems more accurately.
In mothers’ aspects, the current study found that maternal sleep quality significantly improved in the intervention groups. These results could be interpreted as sleep improvement in children through BSIs, and could also enhance maternal sleep quality. In previous studies, it was found that child sleep patterns have a significant correlation with maternal sleep. Covington et al.61 found that total maternal sleep time was associated with mothers’ perception of their toddlers’ sleep problems when sleeping with their toddlers. The number of night awakenings in infants was significantly positively correlated with that of mothers62. Goldman et al.63 examined the association between child and maternal sleep using actigraphy in typically developed children and children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. In this study, they found significant associations between time spent in bed, sleep time, and sleep fragmentation in mothers and children63.
In contrast, BSIs for children did not have a significant effect on maternal depression in the current study. This is inconsistent with the previous SR of psychosocial sleep interventions for children, which found a significant improvement in maternal depression37. Maternal depression could be related to their child’s sleep and the postpartum period. In a previous SR study of postpartum depression in healthy mothers, the overall prevalence of postpartum depression was approximately 12%64. Most postpartum depression is serious in the early postpartum period but decreases and becomes stable over time65,66. That is, because there were more mothers in their early postpartum period who were included in the present study than in the study of Kemplar et al.35, it seems that maternal depression might be related to the postpartum period, not the child’s sleep. In turn, this resulted in an insignificant effect of behavioral sleep intervention on maternal depression. Maternal postpartum depression could also be affected by various factors, such as history of anxiety or depression, prenatal depressive symptoms, and maternal fatigue65,66,67,68. Because of these other factors, maternal depression might not be improved by BSIs for their children.
In addition, the inappropriate use of EPDS for measuring maternal depression has been found in previous RCTs. EPDS was originally developed to measure maternal depression in the postpartum period69, which is defined as the period until 6 months after birth70. However, several studies included in the current study measured maternal depression using EPDS even if the mothers were not in the postpartum period24,42,46. Therefore, maternal depression might not be correctly assessed because of the use of inappropriate measurement tools. Therefore, more research is needed to measure maternal depression using appropriate instruments, and compounders affecting maternal depression should be considered when examining the effect of BSIs for children, on maternal depression. Additionally, future studies are needed to identify the long-term effects of BSIs on maternal depression and changes in maternal depression using a longitudinal study design.
As a result of the current study’s meta-analysis, BSIs, including settling and bedtime routine interventions, can reduce sleep issues in children and improve the mothers’ sleep quality. The application of age-appropriate planned sleep behavior interventions and sleep culture can contribute to the formation of positive sleep habits for both mother and child. It could contribute to early adaptation to the sleep environment and improvement of sleep quality. Therefore, the application of BSI can be implemented when the circadian rhythm is stabilized. Nurses should prepare the BSI plan and provide the mothers with the information soon after childbirth. An accuracy-evaluation of the number of night awakenings using an actigraph is required in future studies, and more studies measuring maternal depression or fatigue using appropriate tools are needed.
The current study has several limitations. First, only ten RCTs were included in the current study because of the limited number of RCTs on child BSIs. Because of the limited number of included studies, we could not perform subgroup analyses according to the ages of the children and the durations of the interventions, which could have helped provide detailed guidelines for clinical practice. Therefore, it is necessary to conduct more RCTs and accumulate evidence on the effects of child BSIs. Second, the current study could not use the data monitored by actigraphy because most studies confirmed sleep patterns using a sleep diary or a sleep-related questionnaire. However, in terms of measuring the sleep quality of children and adults, actigraphy could provide more objective data71,72; thus, future studies need to use actigraphy to achieve and evaluate outcomes of sleep intervention. Third, the current study did not separate participants according to developmental stage (e.g., infants, toddlers) because of the characteristics of the included studies. Since a child’s age could be a confounder of their sleep pattern, future RCTs need to consider child sleep patterns by developmental stage. Fourth, because the duration of interventions and follow-up times were different in most of the included studies, this could cause deviations in the meta-analysis results. Therefore, future SR and meta-analysis studies need to identify the effects of BSIs according to the duration of intervention and follow-up times. Fifth, because the sample size of the control group in Galland’s (2017) study was considered twice, some caution is needed not to overestimate results.
This study found that BSIs in children significantly improved child sleep problems and maternal sleep quality. In general, RCTs of BSIs for children are limited, and therefore, more RCTs need to be conducted to provide further evidence for BSIs. Future SR and meta-analysis studies need to examine the effect of BSIs according to the child’s age, duration of intervention, and outcome measurement time points.
The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Wake, M. et al. Prevalence, stability, and outcomes of cry-fuss and sleep problems in the first 2 years of life: Prospective community-based study. Pediatrics 117, 836–842. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2005-0775 (2006).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Martin, J., Hiscock, H., Hardy, P., Davey, B. & Wake, M. Adverse associations of infant and child sleep problems and parent health: An Australian population study. Pediatrics 119, 947–955. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2006-2569 (2007).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Turnbull, K., Reid, G. J. & Morton, J. B. Behavioral sleep problems and their potential impact on developing executive function in children. Sleep 36, 1077–1084. https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.2814 (2013).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Touchette, E. et al. Associations between sleep duration patterns and behavioral/cognitive functioning at school entry. Sleep 30, 1213–1219. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/30.9.1213 (2007).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Gruber, R. et al. Short sleep duration is associated with poor performance on IQ measures in healthy school-age children. Sleep Med. 11, 289–294. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2009.09.007 (2010).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Smedje, H., Broman, J.-E. & Hetta, J. Associations between disturbed sleep and behavioural difficulties in 635 children aged six to eight years: A study based on parents’ perceptions. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 10, 1–9 (2001).
CAS Article PubMed Google Scholar
Hiscock, H., Canterford, L., Ukoumunne, O. C. & Wake, M. Adverse associations of sleep problems in Australian preschoolers: National population study. Pediatrics 119, 86–93. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2006-1757 (2007).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Carter, P. J., Taylor, B. J., Williams, S. M. & Taylor, R. W. Longitudinal analysis of sleep in relation to BMI and body fat in children: The FLAME study. BMJ 342, d2712. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.d2712 (2011).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Diethelm, K., Bolzenius, K., Cheng, G., Remer, T. & Buyken, A. E. Longitudinal associations between reported sleep duration in early childhood and the development of body mass index, fat mass index and fat free mass index until age 7. Int. J. Pediatr. Obes. 6, e114-123. https://doi.org/10.3109/17477166.2011.566338 (2011).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Giallo, R., Rose, N. & Vittorino, R. Fatigue, wellbeing and parenting in mothers of infants and toddlers with sleep problems. J. Reprod. Infant Psychol. 29, 236–249. https://doi.org/10.1080/02646838.2011.593030 (2011).
Article Google Scholar
Touchette, E. et al. Factors associated with fragmented sleep at night across early childhood. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 159, 242–249. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.159.3.242 (2005).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Touchette, E. et al. Short nighttime sleep-duration and hyperactivity trajectories in early childhood. Pediatrics 124, e985-993. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-2005 (2009).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Senol, D. K., Yurdakul, M. & Ozkan, S. A. The effect of maternal fatigue on breastfeeding. Niger. J. Clin. Pract. 22, 1662–1668. https://doi.org/10.4103/njcp.njcp_576_18 (2019).
CAS Article PubMed Google Scholar
Dickstein, S., Seifer, R. & Albus, K. E. Maternal adult attachment representations across relationship domains and infant outcomes: The importance of family and couple functioning. Attach Hum. Dev 11, 5–27. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730802500164 (2009).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Brown, C. R., Dodds, L., Legge, A., Bryanton, J. & Semenic, S. Factors influencing the reasons why mothers stop breastfeeding. Can. J. Public Health 105, e179-185. https://doi.org/10.17269/cjph.105.4244 (2014).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Lyu, J. et al. Children’s sleep may depend on maternal sleep duration during pregnancy: A retrospective study. Nat. Sci. Sleep 12, 197–207. https://doi.org/10.2147/NSS.S239001 (2020).
ADS Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Tough, S. C. et al. Maternal mental health predicts risk of developmental problems at 3 years of age: Follow up of a community based trial. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 8, 16. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2393-8-16 (2008).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
NINDS Brain Basics: Understanding Sleep https://www.ninds.nih.gov/Disorders/Patient-Caregiver-Education/Understanding-Sleep (2021).
Seron-Ferre, M. et al. Circadian rhythms in the fetus. Mol. Cell. Endocrinol. 349, 68–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mce.2011.07.039 (2012).
CAS Article PubMed Google Scholar
Rivkees, S. A. Developing circadian rhythmicity in infants. Pediatrics 112, 373–381. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.112.2.373 (2003).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Tsai, S. Y., Barnard, K. E., Lentz, M. J. & Thomas, K. A. Mother-infant activity synchrony as a correlate of the emergence of circadian rhythm. Biol. Res. Nurs. 13, 80–88. https://doi.org/10.1177/1099800410378889 (2011).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Matsumoto, K., Shinkoda, H., Kang, M. J. & Seo, Y. J. Longitudinal study of mothers’ sleep-wake behaviors and circadian time patterns from late pregnancy to postpartum – Monitoring of wrist actigraphy and sleep logs. Biol. Rhythm Res. 34, 265–278. https://doi.org/10.1076/brhm.34.3.265.18812 (2003).
Article Google Scholar
Lundqvist-Persson, C. Correlation between level of self-regulation in the newborn infant and developmental status at two years of age. Acta Paediatr. 90, 345–350 (2001).
CAS Article PubMed Google Scholar
Hiscock, H. et al. Improving infant sleep and maternal mental health: A cluster randomised trial. Arch. Dis. Child. 92, 952–958. https://doi.org/10.1136/adc.2006.099812 (2007).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Hiscock, H., Bayer, J. K., Hampton, A., Ukoumunne, O. C. & Wake, M. Long-term mother and child mental health effects of a population-based infant sleep intervention: Cluster-randomized, controlled trial. Pediatrics 122, e621-627. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2007-3783 (2008).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Allen, S. L., Howlett, M. D., Coulombe, J. A. & Corkum, P. V. ABCs of SLEEPING: A review of the evidence behind pediatric sleep practice recommendations. Sleep Med. Rev. 29, 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2015.08.006 (2016).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Mindell, J. A., Leichman, E. S., Lee, C., Williamson, A. A. & Walters, R. M. Implementation of a nightly bedtime routine: How quickly do things improve?. Infant Behav. Dev. 49, 220–227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.09.013 (2017).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
El Rafihi-Ferreira, R., Pires, M. L. N. & de Mattos Silvares, E. F. Behavioral intervention for sleep problems in childhood: A Brazilian randomized controlled trial. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica 32, 5. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41155-019-0118-3 (2019).
Article Google Scholar
Kitsaras, G., Goodwin, M., Allan, J., Kelly, M. P. & Pretty, I. A. Bedtime routines child wellbeing & development. BMC Public Health 18, 386. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-018-5290-3 (2018).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Gradisar, M. et al. Behavioral interventions for infant sleep problems: A randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics 137, e20151486. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2015-1486 (2016).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Mindell, J. A. et al. Efficacy of an internet-based intervention for infant and toddler sleep disturbances. Sleep 34, 451–458. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/34.4.451 (2011).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Mindell, J. A. & Williamson, A. A. Benefits of a bedtime routine in young children: Sleep, development, and beyond. Sleep Med. Rev. 40, 93–108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2017.10.007 (2018).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Douglas, P. S. & Hill, P. S. Behavioral sleep interventions in the first six months of life do not improve outcomes for mothers or infants: A systematic review. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 34, 497–507. https://doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0b013e31829cafa6 (2013).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Page, M. J. et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 372, n71. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.n71 (2021).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Jahan, N., Naveed, S., Zeshan, M. & Tahir, M. A. How to conduct a systematic review: A narrative literature review. Cureus 8, e864. https://doi.org/10.7759/cureus.864 (2016).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Kempler, L., Sharpe, L., Miller, C. B. & Bartlett, D. J. Do psychosocial sleep interventions improve infant sleep or maternal mood in the postnatal period? A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Sleep Med. Rev. 29, 15–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2015.08.002 (2016).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Bryanton, J., Beck, C. T. & Montelpare, W. Postnatal parental education for optimizing infant general health and parent-infant relationships. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD004068.pub4 (2013).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Sterne, J. A. C. et al. RoB 2: A revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ 366, l4898. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.l4898 (2019).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Higgins, J. P. T. & Cochrane, C. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions (2020).
Paul, I. M. et al. INSIGHT responsive parenting intervention and infant sleep. Pediatrics 138, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-0762 (2016).
Article Google Scholar
Galland, B. C. et al. Anticipatory guidance to prevent infant sleep problems within a randomised controlled trial: Infant, maternal and partner outcomes at 6 months of age. BMJ Open 7, e014908. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2016-014908 (2017).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Hiscock, H. & Wake, M. Randomised controlled trial of behavioural infant sleep intervention to improve infant sleep and maternal mood. BMJ 324, 1062–1065. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.324.7345.1062 (2002).
CAS Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Tufanaru, C., Munn, Z., Stephenson, M. & Aromataris, E. Fixed or random effects meta-analysis? Common methodological issues in systematic reviews of effectiveness. JBI Evidence Implem. 13, 196–207. https://doi.org/10.1097/xeb.0000000000000065 (2015).
Article Google Scholar
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. & Rothstein, H. R. A basic introduction to fixed-effect and random-effects models for meta-analysis. Res. Synth. Methods 1, 97–111. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.12 (2010).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Riley, R. D., Higgins, J. P. & Deeks, J. J. Interpretation of random effects meta-analyses. BMJ 342, d549. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.d549 (2011).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Mindell, J. A., Lee, C. I., Leichman, E. S. & Rotella, K. N. Massage-based bedtime routine: Impact on sleep and mood in infants and mothers. Sleep Med. 41, 51–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2017.09.010 (2018).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Mindell, J. A., Telofski, L. S., Wiegand, B. & Kurtz, E. S. A nightly bedtime routine: Impact on sleep in young children and maternal mood. Sleep 32, 599–606. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/32.5.599 (2009).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Rouzafzoon, M., Farnam, F. & Khakbazan, Z. The effects of infant behavioural sleep interventions on maternal sleep and mood, and infant sleep: A randomised controlled trial. J. Sleep Res. 30, e13344. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.13344 (2021).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Stremler, R. et al. Effect of behavioural-educational intervention on sleep for primiparous women and their infants in early postpartum: Multisite randomised controlled trial. BMJ 346, f1164. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.f1164 (2013).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Yu, X. et al. Emergence of racial/ethnic and socioeconomic differences in objectively measured sleep-wake patterns in early infancy: Results of the Rise & SHINE study. Sleep 44, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsaa193 (2021).
Article Google Scholar
Perpetuo, C., Fernandes, M. & Verissimo, M. Comparison between actigraphy records and parental reports of child’s sleep. Front. Pediatr. 8, 567390. https://doi.org/10.3389/fped.2020.567390 (2020).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Williams, K. E., Berthelsen, D., Walker, S. & Nicholson, J. M. A developmental cascade model of behavioral sleep problems and emotional and attentional self-regulation across early childhood. Behav. Sleep Med. 15, 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1080/15402002.2015.1065410 (2017).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Williamson, A. A., Mindell, J. A., Hiscock, H. & Quach, J. Child sleep behaviors and sleep problems from infancy to school-age. Sleep Med. 63, 5–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2019.05.003 (2019).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Mindell, J. A. et al. Development of infant and toddler sleep patterns: Real-world data from a mobile application. J. Sleep Res. 25, 508–516. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12414 (2016).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Grimes, M., Camerota, M. & Propper, C. B. Neighborhood deprivation predicts infant sleep quality. Sleep Health 5, 148–151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleh.2018.11.001 (2019).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Brown, A. & Harries, V. Infant sleep and night feeding patterns during later infancy: Association with breastfeeding frequency, daytime complementary food intake, and infant weight. Breastfeed. Med. 10, 246–252. https://doi.org/10.1089/bfm.2014.0153 (2015).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Lukowski, A. F., Liu, X., Peirano, P., Odio, M. & Bauer, P. J. Disposable diaper use promotes consolidated nighttime sleep and positive mother-infant interactions in Chinese 6-month-olds. J. Fam. Psychol. 29, 371–381. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0000072 (2015).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Ramamurthy, M. B. et al. Effect of current breastfeeding on sleep patterns in infants from Asia-Pacific region. J. Paediatr. Child Health 48, 669–674. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1754.2012.02453.x (2012).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Li, L. et al. Similarities and differences between actigraphy and parent-reported sleep in a Hispanic and non-Hispanic White sample. Sleep Med. 83, 160–167. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2021.04.036 (2021).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Mazza, S., Bastuji, H. & Rey, A. E. Objective and subjective assessments of sleep in children: Comparison of actigraphy, sleep diary completed by children and parents’ estimation. Front. Psychiatry 11, 495. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00495 (2020).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Covington, L. B., Armstrong, B. & Black, M. M. Perceived toddler sleep problems, co-sleeping, and maternal sleep and mental health. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 39, 238–245. https://doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0000000000000535 (2018).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Sinai, D. & Tikotzky, L. Infant sleep, parental sleep and parenting stress in families of mothers on maternity leave and in families of working mothers. Infant Behav. Dev. 35, 179–186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.01.006 (2012).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Goldman, S. E., Wang, L. & Fawkes, D. B. Concordance of mother/child sleep patterns using actigraphy: Preliminary findings. J. Sleep Disord. Treat. Care 3, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.4172/2325-9639.1000133 (2014).
Article Google Scholar
Shorey, S. et al. Prevalence and incidence of postpartum depression among healthy mothers: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Psychiatr. Res. 104, 235–248. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.08.001 (2018).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
McCall-Hosenfeld, J. S., Phiri, K., Schaefer, E., Zhu, J. & Kjerulff, K. Trajectories of depressive symptoms throughout the peri- and postpartum period: Results from the first baby study. J. Womens Health (Larchmt.) 25, 1112–1121. https://doi.org/10.1089/jwh.2015.5310 (2016).
Article Google Scholar
Drozd, F., Haga, S. M., Valla, L. & Slinning, K. Latent trajectory classes of postpartum depressive symptoms: A regional population-based longitudinal study. J. Affect. Disord. 241, 29–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2018.07.081 (2018).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Hall, W. A., Moynihan, M., Bhagat, R. & Wooldridge, J. Relationships between parental sleep quality, fatigue, cognitions about infant sleep, and parental depression pre and post-intervention for infant behavioral sleep problems. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 17, 104. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12884-017-1284-x (2017).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Silverman, M. E. et al. The risk factors for postpartum depression: A population-based study. Depress. Anxiety 34, 178–187. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22597 (2017).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Cox, J. L., Holden, J. M. & Sagovsky, R. Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10-item Edinburgh postnatal depression scale. Br. J. Psychiatry 150, 782–786. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.150.6.782 (1987).
CAS Article PubMed Google Scholar
Miller, L. J. Postpartum depression. JAMA 287, 762–765. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.287.6.762 (2002).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Rudzik, A. E. F., Robinson-Smith, L. & Ball, H. L. Discrepancies in maternal reports of infant sleep vs. actigraphy by mode of feeding. Sleep Med. 49, 90–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2018.06.010 (2018).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Campanini, M. Z. et al. Agreement between sleep diary and actigraphy in a highly educated Brazilian population. Sleep Med. 35, 27–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2017.04.004 (2017).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Download references
This work was supported by 2019 Faculty-Student Research Fund granted from the Mo-Im Kim Nursing Research Institute, College of Nursing at Yonsei University (Number: 6-2019-0040). Kyoungjin Lee received a scholarship from Brain Korea 21 FOUR Project funded by National Research Foundation (NRF) of Korea, Yonsei University College of Nursing (No: F21JB7504007).
Mo-Im Kim Nursing Research Institute, College of Nursing, Yonsei University, 616 College of Nursing, Yonsei-ro 50, Seodaemun-gu, Seoul, 03722, Korea
Jeongok Park, Soo Yeon Kim & Kyoungjin Lee
College of Nursing and Brain Korea 21 FOUR Project, Yonsei University, Yonsei-ro 50, Seodaemun-gu, Seoul, 03722, Korea
Kyoungjin Lee
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
J.P. and K.L. contributed to the conception, design, data collection, data quality assessment, statistical analysis, data interpretation, and manuscript drafting. S.K. contributed to data quality assessment and data interpretation, and manuscript drafting. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript and agreed for all.
Correspondence to Kyoungjin Lee.
The authors declare no competing interests.
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
Reprints and Permissions
Park, J., Kim, S.Y. & Lee, K. Effectiveness of behavioral sleep interventions on children’s and mothers’ sleep quality and maternal depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sci Rep 12, 4172 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-07762-8
Download citation
Received: 27 October 2021
Accepted: 23 February 2022
Published: 09 March 2022
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-07762-8
Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:
Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.
Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.
Advertisement
Advanced search
© 2022 Springer Nature Limited
Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.